Show Menu
Cheatography

Encoding, Storage and Retrieval Cheat Sheet (DRAFT) by

Psychologists discern between three necessary stages in the learning and memory process: encoding, storage, and retrieval. Encoding is defined as the initial learning of information; storage refers to maintaining information over time; retrieval is the ability to access information when you need it.

This is a draft cheat sheet. It is a work in progress and is not finished yet.

Encoding

Encoding refers to the experience of gaining inform­ation.

Principles of encoding
- selective: We pay attention to some things in our enviro­nment and we ignore others.
- prolific: We are always encoding the events of our lives and trying to understand them.

Psycho­logist pinpointed distin­cti­veness—having an event stand out as unusual from a background of similar events—as a key to rememb­ering events.
Example: we might not remember the exact details of how our walk to work was a few weeks after it happened, as it is a mundane and routine event. But, if there was a car crash that happened on that day, we are more likely to remember the details of the event on that particular day.

In addition, when vivid memories are influenced by strong emotional content, they often leave a permanent mark on us. The term flashbulb memory is used to describe this phenom­enon. It refers to how some memories seem to be captured in the mind like a flash photog­raph; because of the emotio­nality of the news and seem to become perman­ently etched in the mind compared to other memories.

As noted above, the process of encoding is selective. In complex situat­ions, very few details are noticed and encoded. The process of encoding always involves recodi­ng—that is, taking the inform­ation from the form it is delivered to us and then converting it in a way that we can unders­tand.

In conclu­sion, encoding is essential in the learning and memory process. However, just because an event is encoded (even if it is encoded well), there’s no guarantee that it will be remembered later.
 

Storage

In order to store memories, the brain bioche­mically alters itself.

For psycho­log­ists, the term memory trace simply refers to the physical change in the nervous system that represents our experi­ence.

Memory traces are not like videos or audio record­ings. We often have errors in our memory, which would not exist if memory traces were perfect recordings of inform­ation. Memory is a creation of what you actually recall and what you believe happened. In a phrase, rememb­ering is recons­tru­ctive (we recons­truct our past with the help of memory traces), not reprod­uctive (a perfect recreation of the past)

Psycho­logists refer to the time between learning and testing as the retention interval. Memories can consol­idate during that time, aiding retention. However, experi­ences can also occur that undermine the memory.

Retroa­ctive interf­erence is one of the main causes of forget­ting. Retroa­ctive interf­erence refers to new activities during the retention interval that interfere with retrieving the specific, older memory. For example, if you witnessed a car crash but hear people describing it from their own perspe­ctive, this new inform­ation may disrupt your own personal memory of the crash.
 

Retrieval

Available inform­ation is the inform­ation that is stored in memory, but we dont know how much and what types are stored. All we can know is what inform­ation we can retrieve—accessible inform­ation.

Accessible inform­ation represents only a tiny slice of the inform­ation available in our brains. Most people have had the experience of trying to remember some fact or event, giving up, and then it comes to them at a later time, even after they’ve stopped trying to remember it. Similarly, if we are given several choices (as in a multip­le-­choice test), we are easily able to recognize the thing are trying to remember.

The general principle that underlies the effect­iveness of retrieval cues is the encoding specif­icity principle,which is an occurrence when retrieval cues overlap the memory trace of an experi­ence.

However, for a retrieval cue to be effective, a match must exist between the cue and the desired target memory. To produce the best retrieval, the cue-target relati­onship should be distin­ctive.