Show Menu
Cheatography

attention processes, models

Defini­tions

Selective Attention
Defini­tion: Focusing on one stimulus while ignoring others.
 
Example: Roger focuses on his math homework and ignores the people talking nearby.
Distra­ction
Defini­tion: When an irrelevant stimulus interferes with the processing of a current task.
 
Example: While playing a game on his phone (less demand­ing), the conver­sation becomes distra­cting
Divided Attention
Defini­tion: Paying attention to more than one thing at once.
 
Example: Roger consci­ously eavesdrops on the conver­sation while playing the game
Attent­ional Capture
Defini­tion: A sudden, involu­ntary shift in attention caused by a salient stimulus.
 
Example: A loud noise from an overturned book cart draws Roger’s attention
Visual Scanning
Defini­tion: Actively moving the eyes to search for or attend to different visual inform­ation.
 
Example: Roger looks from face to face trying to identify people involved in the commotion.
Attention
Attention is the mental process of selecting certain stimuli while ignoring others, allowing us to process relevant inform­ation effici­ently.
 
It’s not a unitary concep­t—a­tte­ntion has multiple forms and underlying processes.
William James
"My experience is what I agree to attend to... It implies withdrawal from some things in order to deal effect­ively with others­"
Broadb­ent’s Filter Model:
We filter incoming stimuli early in proces­sing.
Treisman’s Attenu­ation Theory:
We don’t completely block ignored stimuli; we just turn down their "­vol­ume."
Late Selection Models:
All info is processed somewhat before selection occurs.
Cocktail Party Effect:
Even in a noisy enviro­nment, we can tune in to one conver­sation while filtering others

Types of Attention

Attention
Definition
Example
Selective Attention
Focusing on one stimulus while ignoring others.
Roger solving math problems while ignoring people talking nearby.
Divided Attention
Paying attention to multiple stimul­i/tasks at the same time.
Listening in on a conver­sation while playing a game on the phone.
Sustained Attention
Mainta­ining focus on a task over an extended period of time.
Reading a textbook for an hour without getting distra­cted.
Altern­ating Attention
Shifting focus back and forth between tasks.
Checking a text message and then returning to writing an essay.
Attent­ional Capture
When attention is involu­ntarily drawn to a sudden stimulus.
A loud crash from a fallen book cart pulls Roger’s attention from the conver­sation.
Visual Attention
Focusing on object­s/l­oca­tions in the visual field.
Scanning faces across a room to find someone you recognize.
Auditory Attention
Focusing on specific sounds in the enviro­nment.
Tuning into one voice at a noisy party (cocktail party effect).
Exogenous Attention
Attention driven by an external stimulus (botto­m-up).
A flashing light or sudden noise grabbing your attention automa­tic­ally.
Endogenous Attention
Attention directed by internal goals or intentions (top-d­own).
Intent­ionally looking for your friend in a crowd.
Focused
Concen­trating on a single task with high precision.
Solving a tough math problem without any background noise or interr­upt­ions.
Executive Attention
Contro­lling attention to manage conflict or distra­ctions; often tied to executive functions.
Ignoring a pop-up while trying to submit an online exam.

Attention as Inform­ation Processing

Modern attention research began in the 1950s.
One of the first influe­ntial models: Broadb­ent’s Filter Model (1958).
Based on dichotic listening experi­ments, especially those by Colin Cherry and Neville Moray.
🎧 Key Backgr­ound: Dichotic Listening
Dichotic listening: Different auditory messages are presented to each ear.
Partic­ipant is instructed to focus on one ear only (the attended ear) and shadow the message (repeat it out loud).
💬 Findings:
Partic­ipants could shadow the attended message easily. They could identify the gender of the voice in the unattended ear. But they couldn’t recall content of the unattended message. Even a word repeated 35 times in the unattended ear went unnoticed
Broadb­ent’s Filter Model of Attention (1958)
A theore­tical model that explains how we attend to one message and filter out others.
Stages of Processing
Sensory Memory: Holds all incoming info for a fraction of a second (like a buffer) and sends it to the filter.
Filter: Selects the attended message based on physical charac­ter­istics (e.g., pitch, tone, speed, accent). All other input is filtered out.
Detector: Analyzes the meaning of the filter­ed/­att­ended message. Higher­-level processing happens here.
Short-Term Memory (STM): Receives output from the detector. Holds info for 10–15 seconds, and can pass it on to Long-Term Memory (LTM).
💡 Key Charac­ter­istics of Broadb­ent’s Model
Early selection model: Filtering happens before meaning is processed.
Only the attended input reaches meaningful analysis; the rest is completely blocked.
Very structured and linear model of attention.
📚 Why This Model Matters
Introduced the "flow diagra­m" approach to cognitive psycho­logy.
Foundation for future attention models like Treisman’s attenu­ation theory and Deutsch & Deutsch’s late selection model.
Helps explain selective attention and why we miss inform­ation we’re not focusing on.

Spotlight Model of Attention

Definition
Proposed by Michael Posner (1980).
Describes visual attention as functi­oning like a spotlight beam.
We can mentally “illum­inate” a region in our visual field to process inform­ation more effici­ent­ly—even without moving our eyes.
Types of Attention in the Spotlight Model
Type
Descri­ption
Overt Attention
Attention where the eyes move to focus on an object or location.
Covert Attention
Attention is shifted mentally without moving the eyes (e.g., eavesd­ropping or "­looking without lookin­g").
Key Features of the Model
Enhanc­ement:
Things inside the spotlight are processed faster and more accura­tely.
Limited scope:
Only a small area is enhanced at any one time—like a narrow beam.
Shiftable:
The spotlight can be moved volunt­arily or automa­tically to different parts of the visual field.
Precedes action:
Often, attention shifts before eye or body movements occur.
Key Experi­ment: Posner Cueing Task (1980)
Goal:
To study how attention shifts even without eye movement (covert attent­ion).
Procedure:
Partic­ipants fixate at the center of a screen.
A cue (arrow or flash) indicates where a target is likely to appear.
After a brief delay, the target appears either where the cue pointed (valid) or in another location (invalid).
Findings:
Faster reaction times for validly cued locations.
Slower responses for invalid cues.
Conclu­sion:
Attention enhances processing even without eye moveme­nt—­sup­porting the spotlight idea.
Bottom-Up vs Top-Down Spotlight Shifts
Type of Shift
Triggered By
Bottom-Up
Stimul­us-­driven (salient color, motion, etc.)
A bright light grabs your attention.
Top-Down
Goal-d­irected or expect­ati­on-­based
You search for your friend in a crowd.
Applic­ations
Driving: Antici­pating where a car might come from.
Gaming: Rapid shifts of covert attention to track enemies off-sc­reen.
Sports: A player focusing attention on multiple elements without shifting gaze.

Feature Integr­ation Theory

Proposed by Anne Treisman (1980).
Explains how we perceive objects as unified wholes rather than separate features.
Core Idea
Perception happens in two stages:
Stage 1: Preatt­entive: Automatic, fast, parallel processing of basic features (e.g., color, shape, size, orient­ation). Happens without attention.
Stage 2: Focused Attention: Attention is used to bind features together into a coherent object. This stage is slower and serial (one item at a time).
Key Experi­ments:
Treisman & Gelade (1980):
Partic­ipants searched for a target (e.g., red "­O") among distra­ctors. Feature search: target differed by one featur­e—fast & automatic.
Conjun­ction search: target shared features with distra­cto­rs—­slower, needs attention.
Key Concepts:
Feature Search: One distinct feature; pops out; parallel proces­sing.
Conjun­ction Search: Multiple shared features; requires focused attention; serial.
Illusory Conjun­ctions: Errors where features from different objects are incorr­ectly combin­ed—­happens when attention is limited.
Why It Matters
Explains how we make sense of complex visual scenes.
Supports attent­ional bottleneck theory­—li­mited capacity for integr­ation.
Useful in fields like UI design, security scanning, and unders­tanding ADHD.
 

Modified Early Selection Models of Attention

📌 Broadb­ent’s Original Model Recap (1958)
Type: Early Selection Model
Filter based on: Physical charac­ter­istics only (e.g., pitch, speed).
Unattended info: Fully filtered out before reaching meaning analysis.
Problem: Couldn’t explain how some unattended info (like your name) gets noticed.
📌 Neville Moray’s Findings (1959) – Challenge to Broadbent
Experi­ment: Dichotic listening + shadowing task.
Result: 1/3 of partic­ipants noticed their own name in the unattended ear.
Implic­ation: Unattended info can be processed for meaning, not just physical features.
Real-world parallel: Cocktail party effect (hearing your name across a noisy room).
📌 “Dear Aunt Jane” Experiment (Gray & Wedder­burn, 1960)
Set-up: Mixed message split across ears:
Attended ear: “Dear 7 Jane”
Unattended ear: “9 Aunt 6”
Result: Partic­ipants reported “Dear Aunt Jane”.
Conclu­sion: They switched attention based on meaning (semantic proces­sing).
This shows: Use of top-down processing (expec­tat­ions, context influence attent­ion).

Broadbent vs Treisman Model

Feature
Broadbent (1958)
Treisman (1964)
Filter type
Rigid, all-or­-no­thing
Flexible attenuator
Unattended info
Completely blocked
Weakened but still analyzed
Message selection
Based only on physical traits
Based on physical, language & meaning
Explains cocktail party effect
❌ No
✅ Yes
Top-down influence
❌ No
✅ Yes (semantic analysis possible)

Lavie’s Load Theory of Attention

Lavie proposed that attention and the ability to filter out distra­ctions depend on two main factors:
🧠 1. Processing Capacity:
This refers to the total amount of cognitive resources a person can use at one time.
Everyone has limited processing capaci­ty—our brain can only handle so much inform­ation at once.
When too many stimuli compete for attention, some are inevitably ignored.
🌀 2. Perceptual Load:
This refers to how demanding a task is on our cognitive system.
Low-load tasks: Simple or well-p­rac­ticed tasks (e.g., identi­fying a letter among all Os)
Use less cognitive capacity
Leave spare attention to process other (even irrele­vant) stimuli
High-load tasks: Complex or unfamiliar tasks (e.g., identi­fying a letter among a mix of different letters)
Use more cognitive capacity
Leave no spare attention, so irrelevant stimuli are filtered out
🔁 Intera­ction: The more demanding the task (high load), the less likely irrelevant inform­ation will be processed.
🔬 Experi­mental Support – Forster & Lavie (2008)
🧪 Task: Visual search for a target letter (X or N)
Easy condition: Target surrounded by identical letters (e.g., all "­o"s) → low load
Hard condition: Target surrounded by a mix of different letters → high load
🔄 Manipu­lation: A distra­ctor, like a cartoon character, appears briefly
Findings:
In low-load tasks, the distractor slows down reaction time.
In high-load tasks, distractor has little or no effect.
Why?
Because low-load tasks don't exhaust processing capacity, there's leftover attention that “spills over” to irrelevant stimuli.
🧪The Stroop Effect – A Special Case
Described by J.R. Stroop (1935)
Task: Name the color of the ink a word is printed in
Easy: Shapes or colored patches → simple color-­naming
Hard: Words printed in incong­ruent ink colors (e.g., “RED” printed in blue ink)
Why the Stroop Effect Occurs:
Reading is a highly automatic process.
The meaning of the word (e.g., “RED”) competes with the goal (saying the ink color “blue”).
Even when we try to ignore the word, it’s processed automa­tic­ally, causing interf­erence.
🧠 Key Point: Even in high-load tasks, well-p­rac­ticed or highly salient distra­ctions (like reading words) can still interfere.
🧠 Everyday Applic­ation:
🧠 Everyday Applic­ation
Playing an easy phone game → get distracted by people talking nearby.
But: a sudden fire alarm or someone saying your name grabs your attention regardless of the task.

Kahneman’s Capacity Model of Attention

Proposed by Daniel Kahneman (1973)
Describes attention as a limited resour­ce—like mental energy.
We can perform multiple tasks only if total demand doesn’t exceed capacity.
Key Features of the Model
Limited Capacity: There’s only so much cognitive “fuel” or mental effort available.
Effort: Attention is linked to how much effort a task requires.
Allocation Policy: Attention is distri­buted based on factors like arousal, intent­ions, and task demands.
Arousal: Higher arousal increases available capacity up to a point (Yerke­s-D­odson Law).
Automatic vs. Contro­lled: Automatic tasks use less attention; controlled tasks use more
Allocation of Attention Depends On:
nduring Dispos­itions: Involu­ntary attention (e.g., loud noises, your name being called)
Momentary Intent­ions: What you're currently trying to do (e.g., studying, driving)
Evaluation of Demands: System judges how much effort is needed per task and allocates attention accord­ingly
Why It’s Important
Highlights how mental effort is limited and how tasks compete for attention.
Helps explain multit­asking, mental fatigue, and task priori­tiz­ation.
Applied in areas like cognitive load theory, human factors, and ergono­mics.
 

Treisman’s Attenu­ation Model of Attention (1964)

Type: Early Selection Model (but more flexible than Broadb­ent’s)
Also called: Leaky Filter Model
Goal: To explain how some unattended inform­ation (like hearing your name) still reaches awaren­ess­—even when attention is directed elsewhere.
🎯 Why Treisman Proposed This Model
Broadb­ent’s Model said unattended info is completely blocked after physical filtering.
But experi­ments (e.g., Moray’s) showed people sometimes hear their name or switch attention based on meaning.
Treisman suggested that unattended info isn’t fully blocked, just weakened.
🔄 How Treisman’s Model Works – Step-b­y-Step
🧱1. Attenuator (Instead of a Filter)
Analyzes input on 3 levels:
Physical charac­ter­istics (pitch, speed, tone), Language (grouping into words/­syl­lab­les), Meaning (seman­tics, logical flow)
Selection is based on what’s needed:
If physical differ­ences are enough (e.g., male vs female voice), attention uses that.
If physical cues aren’t enough, the system uses meaning to separate messages.
✅ Attended message → passes through at full strength
🔉 Unattended messages → are attenuated (weake­ned), but not completely filtered out.
📖2. Dictionary Unit
Contains stored words and meanings (in long-term memory).
Each word has a “thres­hold”:
🔽 Low threshold: needs little input to be activated (e.g., your name, “fire”)
🔼 High threshold: needs stronger signal (e.g., uncommon or unimpo­rtant words)
Even a weak signal from the attenuated stream can activate low-th­reshold words.
Real-World Example
You’re at a party, focused on a friend’s story (attended message). Suddenly you hear your name from a nearby conver­sation (unatt­ended message). ➡ According to Treisman: Your name had a low threshold and got through the attenu­ator, activating your attention.

Late Selection Model of Attention

Late selection models propose that all incoming inform­ation is processed to the level of meaning, and only after this full processing is a message selected for conscious awareness or response.
💡 Core Idea: Selection doesn’t happen at the sensory or physical level (as in early models), but after semantic proces­sing.
🔬 MacKay’s (1973) Experiment – Key Evidence
Setup
Partic­ipants were asked to shadow (repeat aloud) sentences in one ear (attended channel).
Example attended sentence:
“They were throwing stones at the bank.” (Ambig­uous: “bank” could mean riverbank or financial bank)
Simult­ane­ously, a biasing word was presented in the unattended ear: ➤ “money” or “river”
Results
Later, partic­ipants were asked to choose which sentence matched the one they had heard:
“They threw stones at the side of the river”
“They threw stones at the savings and loan associ­ation”
Choice reflected the meaning of the biasing word from the unattended ear:
Heard “money” → chose the bank = financial instit­ution
Heard “river” → chose the bank = riverbank
Partic­ipants were unaware of the biasing words.
📍Concl­usion:
Even unattended input was processed semant­ically (for meaning).
Attention occurs after meaning is proces­sed­—thus, late selection.
🧱 Founda­tional Theorists
Deutsch & Deutsch (1963): Proposed that all stimuli are fully analyzed for meaning, but only one response is made.
Norman (1968): Added the idea of relevance or importance boosting some inputs into awareness.
🧠 Implic­ations of MacKay’s Findings:
Our brain processes more than we’re consci­ously aware of.
Unattended inform­ation can influence decisions, judgments, and behaviors.
Challenges the assumption that attention is necessary for unders­tan­ding.
🔄 Shifting Perspe­ctives: Early vs Late Depends on the Context
There’s no single answer to whether attention is early or late.
It depends on:
🧠 Cognitive resources available
🎯 Task difficulty
🎧 Nature of distra­cting stimuli

Inatte­ntional Blindn­ess­&C­hange Blindness

Inatte­ntional Blindness (IB)
A failure to notice a fully visible but unexpected object because attention is engaged elsewhere.
Occurs when attention is focused on a specific task or object, so other stimuli go unnoticed.
Famous Study
Simons & Chabris (1999) – The “Gorilla” Experiment Task: Count basketball passes. An actor in a gorilla suit walks through the scene. Many partic­ipants don’t notice the gorilla. Shows how focused attent­ion­=bl­indness to the unexpe­cted.
Key Points
Caused by selective attention.
Object is right in front of you, but you miss it.
Not due to visual proble­ms—it's cognitive.
Real-life Examples:
Not noticing a cyclist while texting and walking.
Missing a pedestrian while driving and checking GPS.
Change Blindness (CB)
A failure to notice changes in a visual scene, especially when changes happen during a visual disruption (e.g., blink, cut, saccade).
Even large changes can go unnoticed without focused compar­ison.
Famous Study
Simons & Levin (1998) – Door Study A man asks for direct­ions. While distra­cted, he's replaced by a different person. Many people don’t notice the swap. Shows how we don’t store detailed repres­ent­ations of scenes.
Key Points
We don’t compare pre- and post-c­hange images effect­ively.
Visual memory is limited.
Depends on attention to detail and contin­uity.
 

Comments

No comments yet. Add yours below!

Add a Comment

Your Comment

Please enter your name.

    Please enter your email address

      Please enter your Comment.

          More Cheat Sheets by rentasticco

          Biological Bases of Behaviour Cheat Sheet
          Learning Cheat Sheet