\documentclass[10pt,a4paper]{article} % Packages \usepackage{fancyhdr} % For header and footer \usepackage{multicol} % Allows multicols in tables \usepackage{tabularx} % Intelligent column widths \usepackage{tabulary} % Used in header and footer \usepackage{hhline} % Border under tables \usepackage{graphicx} % For images \usepackage{xcolor} % For hex colours %\usepackage[utf8x]{inputenc} % For unicode character support \usepackage[T1]{fontenc} % Without this we get weird character replacements \usepackage{colortbl} % For coloured tables \usepackage{setspace} % For line height \usepackage{lastpage} % Needed for total page number \usepackage{seqsplit} % Splits long words. %\usepackage{opensans} % Can't make this work so far. Shame. Would be lovely. \usepackage[normalem]{ulem} % For underlining links % Most of the following are not required for the majority % of cheat sheets but are needed for some symbol support. \usepackage{amsmath} % Symbols \usepackage{MnSymbol} % Symbols \usepackage{wasysym} % Symbols %\usepackage[english,german,french,spanish,italian]{babel} % Languages % Document Info \author{rentasticco} \pdfinfo{ /Title (learning.pdf) /Creator (Cheatography) /Author (rentasticco) /Subject (Learning Cheat Sheet) } % Lengths and widths \addtolength{\textwidth}{6cm} \addtolength{\textheight}{-1cm} \addtolength{\hoffset}{-3cm} \addtolength{\voffset}{-2cm} \setlength{\tabcolsep}{0.2cm} % Space between columns \setlength{\headsep}{-12pt} % Reduce space between header and content \setlength{\headheight}{85pt} % If less, LaTeX automatically increases it \renewcommand{\footrulewidth}{0pt} % Remove footer line \renewcommand{\headrulewidth}{0pt} % Remove header line \renewcommand{\seqinsert}{\ifmmode\allowbreak\else\-\fi} % Hyphens in seqsplit % This two commands together give roughly % the right line height in the tables \renewcommand{\arraystretch}{1.3} \onehalfspacing % Commands \newcommand{\SetRowColor}[1]{\noalign{\gdef\RowColorName{#1}}\rowcolor{\RowColorName}} % Shortcut for row colour \newcommand{\mymulticolumn}[3]{\multicolumn{#1}{>{\columncolor{\RowColorName}}#2}{#3}} % For coloured multi-cols \newcolumntype{x}[1]{>{\raggedright}p{#1}} % New column types for ragged-right paragraph columns \newcommand{\tn}{\tabularnewline} % Required as custom column type in use % Font and Colours \definecolor{HeadBackground}{HTML}{333333} \definecolor{FootBackground}{HTML}{666666} \definecolor{TextColor}{HTML}{333333} \definecolor{DarkBackground}{HTML}{46704D} \definecolor{LightBackground}{HTML}{F3F6F3} \renewcommand{\familydefault}{\sfdefault} \color{TextColor} % Header and Footer \pagestyle{fancy} \fancyhead{} % Set header to blank \fancyfoot{} % Set footer to blank \fancyhead[L]{ \noindent \begin{multicols}{3} \begin{tabulary}{5.8cm}{C} \SetRowColor{DarkBackground} \vspace{-7pt} {\parbox{\dimexpr\textwidth-2\fboxsep\relax}{\noindent \hspace*{-6pt}\includegraphics[width=5.8cm]{/web/www.cheatography.com/public/images/cheatography_logo.pdf}} } \end{tabulary} \columnbreak \begin{tabulary}{11cm}{L} \vspace{-2pt}\large{\bf{\textcolor{DarkBackground}{\textrm{Learning Cheat Sheet}}}} \\ \normalsize{by \textcolor{DarkBackground}{rentasticco} via \textcolor{DarkBackground}{\uline{cheatography.com/177906/cs/46121/}}} \end{tabulary} \end{multicols}} \fancyfoot[L]{ \footnotesize \noindent \begin{multicols}{3} \begin{tabulary}{5.8cm}{LL} \SetRowColor{FootBackground} \mymulticolumn{2}{p{5.377cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Cheatographer}} \\ \vspace{-2pt}rentasticco \\ \uline{cheatography.com/rentasticco} \\ \end{tabulary} \vfill \columnbreak \begin{tabulary}{5.8cm}{L} \SetRowColor{FootBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{p{5.377cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Cheat Sheet}} \\ \vspace{-2pt}Published 15th April, 2025.\\ Updated 15th April, 2025.\\ Page {\thepage} of \pageref{LastPage}. \end{tabulary} \vfill \columnbreak \begin{tabulary}{5.8cm}{L} \SetRowColor{FootBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{p{5.377cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Sponsor}} \\ \SetRowColor{white} \vspace{-5pt} %\includegraphics[width=48px,height=48px]{dave.jpeg} Measure your website readability!\\ www.readability-score.com \end{tabulary} \end{multicols}} \begin{document} \raggedright \raggedcolumns % Set font size to small. Switch to any value % from this page to resize cheat sheet text: % www.emerson.emory.edu/services/latex/latex_169.html \footnotesize % Small font. \begin{multicols*}{2} \begin{tabularx}{8.4cm}{x{3.04 cm} x{4.96 cm} } \SetRowColor{DarkBackground} \mymulticolumn{2}{x{8.4cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Definitions}} \tn % Row 0 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} Learning & A relatively permanent change in behaviour brought about by experience or practice. \tn % Row Count 4 (+ 4) % Row 1 \SetRowColor{white} & "Relatively permanent"-{}- learning causes physical changes in the brain to record what has been learned. \tn % Row Count 9 (+ 5) % Row 2 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} Maturation & Change that is accomplished through biological growth, controlled by a genetic blueprint \tn % Row Count 13 (+ 4) % Row 3 \SetRowColor{white} Habituation & A decline in the response to a stimulus once the stimulus has become familiar. \tn % Row Count 17 (+ 4) % Row 4 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \seqsplit{Dis-habituation} & An increase in responsiveness when something novel is presented, following a series of presentations of something familiar. \tn % Row Count 23 (+ 6) % Row 5 \SetRowColor{white} Classical Conditioning & A form of learning in which one stimulus is paired with another so that the organism learns a relationship (implicit and involuntary) between the stimuli. \tn % Row Count 30 (+ 7) \end{tabularx} \par\addvspace{1.3em} \vfill \columnbreak \begin{tabularx}{8.4cm}{x{3.04 cm} x{4.96 cm} } \SetRowColor{DarkBackground} \mymulticolumn{2}{x{8.4cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Definitions (cont)}} \tn % Row 6 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} Unconditioned Stimulus & A stimulus that reliably triggers a particular response without prior training. \tn % Row Count 4 (+ 4) % Row 7 \SetRowColor{white} Unconditioned Response & A response elicited by an unconditioned stimulus without prior training. \tn % Row Count 7 (+ 3) % Row 8 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} Conditioned Stimulus & An initially neutral stimulus that comes to elicit a new response due to pairings with the unconditioned stimulus. \tn % Row Count 12 (+ 5) % Row 9 \SetRowColor{white} Conditioned Response & A response elicited by an initially neutral stimulus—the conditioned stimulus (CS)—after it has been paired repeatedly with an unconditioned stimulus (US). \tn % Row Count 19 (+ 7) % Row 10 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} Second Order Conditioning & A form of learning in which a neutral stimulus is first made meaningful through classical conditioning. Then, that stimulus (the CS) is paired with a new, neutral stimulus until the new stimulus also elicits the conditioned response. \tn % Row Count 29 (+ 10) % Row 11 \SetRowColor{white} Extinction & The weakening of a learned response that is produced if a conditioned stimulus is now repeatedly presented without the unconditioned stimulus. \tn % Row Count 35 (+ 6) \end{tabularx} \par\addvspace{1.3em} \vfill \columnbreak \begin{tabularx}{8.4cm}{x{3.04 cm} x{4.96 cm} } \SetRowColor{DarkBackground} \mymulticolumn{2}{x{8.4cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Definitions (cont)}} \tn % Row 12 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} Spontaneous Recovery & The reappearance of an extinguished response after a period in which no further conditioning trials have been presented. \tn % Row Count 5 (+ 5) % Row 13 \SetRowColor{white} Stimulus Generalization & The tendency for stimuli similar to those used during learning to elicit a reaction similar to the learned response. \tn % Row Count 10 (+ 5) % Row 14 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} Discrimination & An aspect of learning in which the organism learns to respond differently to stimuli that have been associated with a US (or reiforcement), and stimuli that have not. \tn % Row Count 17 (+ 7) % Row 15 \SetRowColor{white} Inhibitor & A stimulus signaling that an event is not coming, which elicits a response opposite to the one that the event usually elicits. \tn % Row Count 23 (+ 6) % Row 16 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} Blocking Effect & A result showing that an animal learns nothing about a stimulus if the stimulus provides no new information. \tn % Row Count 28 (+ 5) % Row 17 \SetRowColor{white} Compensatory Response & A response that offsets the effects of the upcoming unconditioned stimulus. \tn % Row Count 32 (+ 4) \end{tabularx} \par\addvspace{1.3em} \vfill \columnbreak \begin{tabularx}{8.4cm}{x{3.04 cm} x{4.96 cm} } \SetRowColor{DarkBackground} \mymulticolumn{2}{x{8.4cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Definitions (cont)}} \tn % Row 18 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} Instrumental Conditioning & A form of learning in which the participant receives a reinforcer only after performing the desired response, and thereby learns a relationship between the response and the reinforcer. (Voluntary, explicit) \tn % Row Count 9 (+ 9) % Row 19 \SetRowColor{white} Law of Effect & Thorndike's theory that a response followed by a reward will be strengthened, whereas a response followed by no reward (or by punishment) will be weakened. \tn % Row Count 16 (+ 7) % Row 20 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} Premack Principle & states that more probable behaviors will reinforce less probable behaviors. \tn % Row Count 20 (+ 4) % Row 21 \SetRowColor{white} Operant & In Skinner's system, an instrumental response that is defined by its effect (the way it operates) on the environment. \tn % Row Count 25 (+ 5) % Row 22 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} Reinforcer & A stimulus delivered after a response that makes the response more likely in the future. \tn % Row Count 29 (+ 4) % Row 23 \SetRowColor{white} Shaping & The process of eliciting a desired response by rewarding behaviors that are increasingly similar to that response. \tn % Row Count 34 (+ 5) \end{tabularx} \par\addvspace{1.3em} \vfill \columnbreak \begin{tabularx}{8.4cm}{x{3.04 cm} x{4.96 cm} } \SetRowColor{DarkBackground} \mymulticolumn{2}{x{8.4cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Definitions (cont)}} \tn % Row 24 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} Behavioural Contrast & A response pattern in which an organism evalu- ates a reward relative to other avail- able rewards or those that have been available recently. \tn % Row Count 6 (+ 6) % Row 25 \SetRowColor{white} Partial Reinforcement & A learning condition in which only some of the organism's responses are reinforced \tn % Row Count 10 (+ 4) % Row 26 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} Schedule of Reinforcement & The rules about how often and under what conditions a response will be reinforced. \tn % Row Count 14 (+ 4) % Row 27 \SetRowColor{white} Ratio Schedule & A pattern of delivering reinforcements only after a certain number of responses. \tn % Row Count 18 (+ 4) % Row 28 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} Interval Schedule & A pattern of delivering reinforcements only after a certain amount of time has passed. \tn % Row Count 22 (+ 4) % Row 29 \SetRowColor{white} Latent Learning & Learning that occurs without a corresponding change in behavior. \tn % Row Count 25 (+ 3) % Row 30 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} Learned Helplessness & A condition of passivity apparently created by expo- sure to inescapable aversive events. This condition inhibits or prevents learning in later situations in which escape or avoidance is possible. \tn % Row Count 34 (+ 9) \end{tabularx} \par\addvspace{1.3em} \vfill \columnbreak \begin{tabularx}{8.4cm}{x{3.04 cm} x{4.96 cm} } \SetRowColor{DarkBackground} \mymulticolumn{2}{x{8.4cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Definitions (cont)}} \tn % Row 31 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} Observational Learning & The process of watching how others behave and learning from their example. \tn % Row Count 4 (+ 4) % Row 32 \SetRowColor{white} Vicarious Conditioning & A form of learning in which the learner acquires a conditioned response merely by observing another participant being conditioned. \tn % Row Count 10 (+ 6) % Row 33 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} Mirror Neurons & Neurons that fire whenever an animal performs an action, such as stretching out its arm or reaching toward a target, and also whenever the animal watches another performing the same action. \tn % Row Count 18 (+ 8) % Row 34 \SetRowColor{white} Taste Aversion Learning & A form of learning in which an organism learns to avoid a taste after just one pairing of that taste with illness. \tn % Row Count 23 (+ 5) % Row 35 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} Prepared Learning & Learning that occurs without extensive training because of an evolved predisposition to the behavior. \tn % Row Count 28 (+ 5) % Row 36 \SetRowColor{white} Presynaptic Facilitation & A process, documented in studies of Aplysia, that underlies many kinds of learning. It occurs when learning results in an increased release of neurotransmitter into the synapse. \tn % Row Count 36 (+ 8) \end{tabularx} \par\addvspace{1.3em} \vfill \columnbreak \begin{tabularx}{8.4cm}{x{3.04 cm} x{4.96 cm} } \SetRowColor{DarkBackground} \mymulticolumn{2}{x{8.4cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Definitions (cont)}} \tn % Row 37 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} Long Term Potentiation & A long-lasting increase in a neuron's response to specific inputs, caused by repeated stimulation. \tn % Row Count 5 (+ 5) \hhline{>{\arrayrulecolor{DarkBackground}}--} \end{tabularx} \par\addvspace{1.3em} \begin{tabularx}{8.4cm}{X} \SetRowColor{DarkBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Classical Conditioning}} \tn % Row 0 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Ivan Pavlov: Won the nobel prize in 1904 for his work on digestive physiology: studying salivation in dogs} \tn % Row Count 3 (+ 3) % Row 1 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Discovery of Classical Conditioning} \tn % Row Count 4 (+ 1) % Row 2 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Noticed that dogs began to salivate not just at food (dry food = natural salivation), but also at stimuli associated with food (like the dish, the person, or the lab). These neutral stimuli began to trigger salivation after being repeatedly paired with food.} \tn % Row Count 10 (+ 6) % Row 3 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Experiment Setup} \tn % Row Count 11 (+ 1) % Row 4 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Pavlov rang a bell (neutral stimulus) before giving food. After several repetitions, the bell alone triggered salivation.} \tn % Row Count 14 (+ 3) % Row 5 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Unconditioned Stimulus (US): Food – naturally causes a reaction.} \tn % Row Count 16 (+ 2) % Row 6 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Unconditioned Response (UR): Salivation to food – natural, unlearned.} \tn % Row Count 18 (+ 2) % Row 7 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Conditioned Stimulus (CS): Bell – originally neutral, becomes meaningful through pairing.} \tn % Row Count 20 (+ 2) % Row 8 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Conditioned Response (CR): Salivation to the bell – learned response.} \tn % Row Count 22 (+ 2) % Row 9 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Acquisition of Conditioned Responses} \tn % Row Count 23 (+ 1) % Row 10 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{At first, a Conditioned Stimulus (CS) (like a bell) does not cause a Conditioned Response (CR) (like salivation). After repeated pairings with the Unconditioned Stimulus (US) (like food), the CS starts to trigger the CR. Learning is gradual—the strength of the CR builds up over time with more CS-US pairings.} \tn % Row Count 30 (+ 7) \end{tabularx} \par\addvspace{1.3em} \vfill \columnbreak \begin{tabularx}{8.4cm}{X} \SetRowColor{DarkBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Classical Conditioning (cont)}} \tn % Row 11 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Second-Order Conditioning} \tn % Row Count 1 (+ 1) % Row 12 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Once a CS (e.g., light) has been paired with a US (e.g., food) to elicit a CR (salivation), a new neutral stimulus (e.g., a bell) can be paired with the CS (light) to also trigger the CR—even without the US. This is called second-order conditioning. Example: If the sight of a dentist causes fear due to painful experiences (US), then related cues (dentist's office, voice, etc.) can also trigger fear. It explains how fears or emotional responses spread through associations.} \tn % Row Count 11 (+ 10) % Row 13 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Extinction} \tn % Row Count 12 (+ 1) % Row 14 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{If the CS is presented without the US repeatedly, the CR gradually weakens and disappears. This is called extinction. Example: If a bell is rung but no food follows, over time, the dog will stop salivating to the bell. Extinction is not the same as forgetting: Forgetting is slow; extinction can happen in just a few trials. Evidence: After a delay (with no exposure), the CR can return (spontaneous recovery).} \tn % Row Count 21 (+ 9) % Row 15 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Spontaneous Recovery} \tn % Row Count 22 (+ 1) % Row 16 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{After extinction, if the animal is given a rest, the CS can again trigger the CR when presented. Shows that extinction doesn't erase the original learning—it just suppresses it. Spontaneous recovery means the memory is still there; the animal is testing whether the CS is informative again.} \tn % Row Count 28 (+ 6) % Row 17 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Reconditioning} \tn % Row Count 29 (+ 1) % Row 18 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{If the animal is conditioned again after extinction, it learns much faster than the first time. Suggests that some memory of the original learning remains.} \tn % Row Count 33 (+ 4) \end{tabularx} \par\addvspace{1.3em} \vfill \columnbreak \begin{tabularx}{8.4cm}{X} \SetRowColor{DarkBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Classical Conditioning (cont)}} \tn % Row 19 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Real-Life Example: Exposure Therapy} \tn % Row Count 1 (+ 1) % Row 20 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Used to treat phobias and anxiety. The feared stimulus (CS) is presented without danger or trauma (US). Over time, anxiety (CR) decreases = extinction. However, after therapy ends, anxiety can return = spontaneous recovery. Not a failure of therapy, just a sign that more sessions are needed.} \tn % Row Count 7 (+ 6) % Row 21 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Generalization} \tn % Row Count 8 (+ 1) % Row 22 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Definition: The tendency of a learned response (CR) to occur in the presence of stimuli that are similar, but not identical, to the original conditioned stimulus (CS).} \tn % Row Count 12 (+ 4) % Row 23 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Example: A dog trained to salivate at a specific tone will also salivate (less strongly) to other, similar tones.} \tn % Row Count 15 (+ 3) % Row 24 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Generalization Gradient: The more different a new stimulus is from the original CS, the weaker the conditioned response becomes.} \tn % Row Count 18 (+ 3) % Row 25 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Discrimination} \tn % Row Count 19 (+ 1) % Row 26 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Definition: The ability to distinguish between different stimuli, responding only to the CS+ (which is followed by the US) and not to similar but non-predictive stimuli (CS–).} \tn % Row Count 23 (+ 4) % Row 27 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Example: If a red light (CS+) signals a boat horn (US), and an orange light (CS–) never does, a person will eventually tense up only to the red light.} \tn % Row Count 27 (+ 4) % Row 28 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{CS– Role: It signals the absence of the US, becoming an inhibitor that reduces the likelihood of the CR.} \tn % Row Count 30 (+ 3) \end{tabularx} \par\addvspace{1.3em} \vfill \columnbreak \begin{tabularx}{8.4cm}{X} \SetRowColor{DarkBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Classical Conditioning (cont)}} \tn % Row 29 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{CS as a "Signal"} \tn % Row Count 1 (+ 1) % Row 30 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{The CS works best when it predicts the arrival of the US.} \tn % Row Count 3 (+ 2) % Row 31 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Timing Matters: Forward pairing (CS before US, short delay): Most effective. Simultaneous pairing (CS and US at the same time): Less effective. Backward pairing (US before CS): Least effective. Analogy: Like a caution sign before a dangerous curve: Just before the curve = effective (CS predicts US). Too early = ineffective (CS too far ahead of US). During or after the curve = useless or confusing (simultaneous or backward pairing).} \tn % Row Count 12 (+ 9) % Row 32 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Contingency vs. Contiguity} \tn % Row Count 13 (+ 1) % Row 33 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Contiguity means the CS (Conditioned Stimulus) and US (Unconditioned Stimulus) occur close in time. Contingency means the CS predicts the likelihood of the US. Key Insight: Learning doesn't happen just because two things happen close together (contiguity); instead, learning depends on whether the CS provides useful information about the US (contingency). 🐶 Example: The dog hears a metronome and gets food. Many other things (light, noise) are also present. But only the metronome reliably signals the arrival of food. That's contingency.} \tn % Row Count 24 (+ 11) % Row 34 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{The Role of Information} \tn % Row Count 25 (+ 1) % Row 35 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Animals (and humans) learn only from stimuli that give reliable info about what's going to happen. If a stimulus is always present, regardless of whether the US comes or not (like light fixtures), it gives no predictive value and won't be learned as a signal.} \tn % Row Count 31 (+ 6) \end{tabularx} \par\addvspace{1.3em} \vfill \columnbreak \begin{tabularx}{8.4cm}{X} \SetRowColor{DarkBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Classical Conditioning (cont)}} \tn % Row 36 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Experiment on Rats – The Role of Predictive Value} \tn % Row Count 2 (+ 2) % Row 37 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Group A: Shock sometimes follows the bell, but it also happens just as often without the bell → no contingency, no learning. Group B: Shock more likely after the bell than without → some contingency, learning occurs. Conclusion: Even an imperfect predictor (40\% chance) can cause conditioning if it increases the likelihood of a US compared to baseline.} \tn % Row Count 10 (+ 8) % Row 38 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{The Absence of Contingency} \tn % Row Count 11 (+ 1) % Row 39 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{When tones and shocks are randomly paired, there's no way to know when a shock is coming → no conditioning happens. If shocks only follow tones, even inconsistently (e.g. 50\% of the time), animals learn because the tone predicts something. Key Concept: Unpredictability leads to chronic stress. When there's a danger signal (e.g. tone), there's also a sense of safety when it's absent. Random shocks = constant anxiety.} \tn % Row Count 20 (+ 9) % Row 40 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Rescorla-Wagner Model} \tn % Row Count 21 (+ 1) % Row 41 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Learning happens when there's a surprise, and it stops when things become predictable.} \tn % Row Count 23 (+ 2) % Row 42 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Core Idea} \tn % Row Count 24 (+ 1) % Row 43 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Your brain is constantly trying to predict what will happen. If something happens that's unexpected, your brain says: "Whoa! I didn't see that coming — I need to learn from this!"} \tn % Row Count 28 (+ 4) % Row 44 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{How It Works} \tn % Row Count 29 (+ 1) % Row 45 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Let's say the brain keeps a "score" of how much it expects the US (e.g., food or shock) after a CS (like a tone). 🧾 The Learning Formula: Change in learning = How surprising the US is = (What actually happened) – (What was expected) Or: ΔV = λ – V Where: ΔV = change in strength of learning λ = the actual outcome (Was there food/shock? How strong?) V = what was expected (How much did the animal already think the food/shock was coming?)} \tn % Row Count 39 (+ 10) \end{tabularx} \par\addvspace{1.3em} \vfill \columnbreak \begin{tabularx}{8.4cm}{X} \SetRowColor{DarkBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Classical Conditioning (cont)}} \tn % Row 46 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Key Points} \tn % Row Count 1 (+ 1) % Row 47 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Learning = Surprise No surprise = no learning. Prediction gets updated each time based on error (difference between expected and actual outcome). Eventually, when predictions are perfect, learning stops. If a new CS (like a light) adds no new information, it won't be learned — this explains blocking (an advanced concept, but tied to this model).} \tn % Row Count 9 (+ 8) % Row 48 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{CR and UR are NOT always the same} \tn % Row Count 10 (+ 1) % Row 49 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{UR (Unconditioned Response) happens naturally after the US (Unconditioned Stimulus). CR (Conditioned Response) is a learned reaction to the CS (Conditioned Stimulus). They might look different even though they're both "responses."} \tn % Row Count 15 (+ 5) % Row 50 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Example:} \tn % Row Count 16 (+ 1) % Row 51 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Rat + shock (US) → jumps and squeals (UR) Flashing light (CS) → freezes and heart slows (CR = preparing for shock)} \tn % Row Count 19 (+ 3) % Row 52 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{CR = "Get ready!" response Not a direct copy of the UR—it's an anticipatory adjustment.} \tn % Row Count 21 (+ 2) % Row 53 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{CR helps the body prepare} \tn % Row Count 22 (+ 1) % Row 54 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Animals aren't reacting randomly — the CR prepares them for what's coming.} \tn % Row Count 24 (+ 2) % Row 55 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Example: Tone → signals food is coming → dog moistens mouth Light → signals shock → animal freezes, alert 📌 This preparation makes the animal more efficient or safe.} \tn % Row Count 28 (+ 4) % Row 56 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Conditioning and Drug Tolerance} \tn % Row Count 29 (+ 1) % Row 57 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Repeated drug use (like heroin) leads to tolerance: you need more to feel the same effect. Why? Because the body learns to compensate in advance. How? US = heroin UR = drug's biological effects (pain relief, dry mouth, good mood) CS = sight of needle, drug environment CR = opposite of the drug's effects (more pain sensitivity, bad mood, wet mouth) 📌 CR is a compensatory response = the body is trying to stay in balance (homeostasis).} \tn % Row Count 38 (+ 9) \end{tabularx} \par\addvspace{1.3em} \vfill \columnbreak \begin{tabularx}{8.4cm}{X} \SetRowColor{DarkBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Classical Conditioning (cont)}} \tn % Row 58 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Drug Craving = CR with no US} \tn % Row Count 1 (+ 1) % Row 59 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{If a user sees the CS (needle, place, routine) but no drug arrives, the CR still happens. So they feel pain, depression, cravings — the opposite of what the drug would've done. 📌 Craving = your body bracing for a drug that doesn't come.} \tn % Row Count 6 (+ 5) \hhline{>{\arrayrulecolor{DarkBackground}}-} \end{tabularx} \par\addvspace{1.3em} \begin{tabularx}{8.4cm}{x{4 cm} x{4 cm} } \SetRowColor{DarkBackground} \mymulticolumn{2}{x{8.4cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Insight Learning}} \tn % Row 0 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} What is Insight Learning? & Insight learning is a type of learning that happens suddenly, through understanding relationships between different parts of a problem, rather than through trial-and-error. \tn % Row Count 9 (+ 9) % Row 1 \SetRowColor{white} & It involves a sudden realization—an "Aha!" moment—where the solution just clicks. \tn % Row Count 14 (+ 5) % Row 2 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} & Insight is not based on conditioning or reinforcement, but on cognitive restructuring of the problem. \tn % Row Count 20 (+ 6) % Row 3 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{2}{x{8.4cm}}{Background \& Theorist: Wolfgang K{\"o}hler} \tn % Row Count 21 (+ 1) % Row 4 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} & K{\"o}hler was a Gestalt psychologist who studied problem-solving in chimpanzees. \tn % Row Count 25 (+ 4) % Row 5 \SetRowColor{white} & Gestalt psychology emphasizes holistic processing—how we perceive whole patterns, not just bits and pieces. \tn % Row Count 31 (+ 6) \end{tabularx} \par\addvspace{1.3em} \vfill \columnbreak \begin{tabularx}{8.4cm}{x{4 cm} x{4 cm} } \SetRowColor{DarkBackground} \mymulticolumn{2}{x{8.4cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Insight Learning (cont)}} \tn % Row 6 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{2}{x{8.4cm}}{K{\"o}hler's Famous Experiments with Chimps} \tn % Row Count 1 (+ 1) % Row 7 \SetRowColor{white} 🐒 Example 1: Sultan and the Stick Setup: A chimpanzee named Sultan was placed in a cage with a banana just out of reach, and sticks nearby. Process: Sultan tried reaching it unsuccessfully, then stopped and seemed to think. Insight: Suddenly, Sultan used one stick to pull another closer and joined them to reach the banana. 👉 He did not arrive at this through gradual \seqsplit{trial-and-error—it} came suddenly. & 🐒Example 2: Box Stacking Chimps were given boxes and a hanging banana. They stacked the boxes and climbed them to get the banana, showing understanding of spatial relationships. \tn % Row Count 22 (+ 21) % Row 8 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{2}{x{8.4cm}}{Key Characteristics of Insight Learning} \tn % Row Count 23 (+ 1) % Row 9 \SetRowColor{white} Characteristic & Description \tn % Row Count 24 (+ 1) % Row 10 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} Suddenness & The solution appears all at once (the "aha!" experience). \tn % Row Count 28 (+ 4) % Row 11 \SetRowColor{white} Understanding & Involves grasping the structure of the problem, not random attempts. \tn % Row Count 32 (+ 4) \end{tabularx} \par\addvspace{1.3em} \vfill \columnbreak \begin{tabularx}{8.4cm}{x{4 cm} x{4 cm} } \SetRowColor{DarkBackground} \mymulticolumn{2}{x{8.4cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Insight Learning (cont)}} \tn % Row 12 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} No trial-and-error & Unlike conditioning, it doesn't rely on repeated errors or reinforcement. \tn % Row Count 4 (+ 4) % Row 13 \SetRowColor{white} Transferability & The solution or principle can often be applied to new but similar problems. \tn % Row Count 8 (+ 4) % Row 14 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} Requires mental reorganization & Learner reinterprets the problem and mentally restructures the elements \tn % Row Count 12 (+ 4) % Row 15 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{2}{x{8.4cm}}{Insight Learning vs. Trial-and-Error Learning} \tn % Row Count 13 (+ 1) % Row 16 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} Insight Learning & Trial-and-Error Learning \tn % Row Count 15 (+ 2) % Row 17 \SetRowColor{white} Cognitive and sudden & Behavioral and gradual \tn % Row Count 17 (+ 2) % Row 18 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} Based on perception and problem analysis & Based on repeated attempts and failures \tn % Row Count 19 (+ 2) % Row 19 \SetRowColor{white} May take longer to reach, but solution is quick & Gradual improvement over time \tn % Row Count 22 (+ 3) % Row 20 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} Solving a riddle by rethinking it & Trying keys one by one to open a lock \tn % Row Count 24 (+ 2) % Row 21 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{2}{x{8.4cm}}{📌 Implications of Insight Learning} \tn % Row Count 25 (+ 1) % Row 22 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} Shows the Role of Cognition: Learning isn't always about \seqsplit{reinforcement—thinking} matters & Applies to Humans \& Animals: Though common in humans (especially in problem-solving), it has been shown in chimpanzees, birds, and other species. \tn % Row Count 33 (+ 8) \end{tabularx} \par\addvspace{1.3em} \vfill \columnbreak \begin{tabularx}{8.4cm}{x{4 cm} x{4 cm} } \SetRowColor{DarkBackground} \mymulticolumn{2}{x{8.4cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Insight Learning (cont)}} \tn % Row 23 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} Relevance in Education: Encourages the design of learning environments that foster critical thinking rather than rote memorization. Helps explain creative problem-solving in real-life situations. & Real-Life Examples: Figuring out a tricky riddle after staring at it for a while. A child suddenly realizing how to tie shoelaces after watching but not previously succeeding. An inventor seeing a solution to a problem after stepping away from it and then suddenly "seeing" the answer. \tn % Row Count 15 (+ 15) \hhline{>{\arrayrulecolor{DarkBackground}}--} \end{tabularx} \par\addvspace{1.3em} \begin{tabularx}{8.4cm}{X} \SetRowColor{DarkBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Operant/Instrumental Conditioning}} \tn % Row 0 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{What is Instrumental Conditioning?} \tn % Row Count 1 (+ 1) % Row 1 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Also known as Operant Conditioning. Involves learning voluntary behaviors (as opposed to automatic reflexes). Behavior is initiated by the organism, not triggered by an external stimulus. The outcome of the behavior (its consequence) shapes future behavior.} \tn % Row Count 7 (+ 6) % Row 2 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Classical vs. Operant Conditioning} \tn % Row Count 8 (+ 1) % Row 3 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Classical: Involuntary/reflexive responses (e.g., salivation). Instrumental: Voluntary actions (e.g., pressing a lever to get food).} \tn % Row Count 11 (+ 3) % Row 4 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Thorndike and the Law of Effect} \tn % Row Count 12 (+ 1) % Row 5 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Puzzle Box Experiment:} \tn % Row Count 13 (+ 1) % Row 6 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Hungry cats placed in a box with a mechanism (like a loop or lever) to escape and access food. First attempts: random behaviors (biting, scratching). Eventually, by trial and error, they hit the correct action. With repetition, escape time gradually decreased—indicating learning.} \tn % Row Count 19 (+ 6) % Row 7 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Insight or Gradual Learning?} \tn % Row Count 20 (+ 1) % Row 8 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Thorndike found no sudden "Aha!" moment. Learning curve was gradual, not abrupt → suggests no reasoning, just reinforcement.} \tn % Row Count 23 (+ 3) % Row 9 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{The Law of Effect:} \tn % Row Count 24 (+ 1) % Row 10 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Behavior followed by a reward → strengthened. Behavior followed by no reward or punishment → weakened. The animal doesn't need insight or understanding—just consequences. 📌 Learning = responses are "stamped in" (if rewarded) or "stamped out" (if not). 🔁 Parallel to Natural Selection: Like evolution: successful behaviors "survive", and useless ones fade. No conscious direction—just selection based on outcomes.} \tn % Row Count 33 (+ 9) \end{tabularx} \par\addvspace{1.3em} \vfill \columnbreak \begin{tabularx}{8.4cm}{X} \SetRowColor{DarkBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Operant/Instrumental Conditioning (cont)}} \tn % Row 11 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Skinner and Operant Behavior} \tn % Row Count 1 (+ 1) % Row 12 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Key Ideas: Distinguished operant from classical conditioning: Classical: Response is elicited by a stimulus. Operant: Response is emitted voluntarily by the organism. Called these voluntary responses operants because they operate on the environment. 📍 Core principle: Behavior + Positive Consequence = More Likely in Future Behavior + Negative Consequence = Less Likely} \tn % Row Count 9 (+ 8) % Row 13 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{The Skinner Box: A controlled chamber where animals (rats, pigeons) could perform behaviors like pressing a lever or pecking a key for food. Allowed for rapid, repeated trials. Measured response rate = \# of behaviors per unit of time. ✅ Advantage: More efficient than Thorndike's puzzle box (didn't need to reset after each trial).} \tn % Row Count 16 (+ 7) % Row 14 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Differences Between Classical and Instrumental Conditioning} \tn % Row Count 18 (+ 2) % Row 15 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Classical Conditioning: Learning about the relationship between two stimuli (e.g., bell and food). The response is automatic or reflexive (UR). Instrumental Conditioning (Operant Conditioning): Learning about the relationship between a response and its consequence (reinforcer or punishment). The response is voluntary. Despite differences, both involve learning relationships among events and share key phenomena (like extinction, generalization, and discrimination).} \tn % Row Count 28 (+ 10) % Row 16 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Learning Trials and Extinction} \tn % Row Count 29 (+ 1) % Row 17 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{In classical conditioning: CS followed by US leads to learning. In instrumental conditioning: Response followed by reinforcer leads to learning. Extinction happens when reinforcement stops. The behavior gradually weakens or disappears.} \tn % Row Count 34 (+ 5) \end{tabularx} \par\addvspace{1.3em} \vfill \columnbreak \begin{tabularx}{8.4cm}{X} \SetRowColor{DarkBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Operant/Instrumental Conditioning (cont)}} \tn % Row 18 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Generalization and Discrimination Generalization: After learning a response to one stimulus (S+), animals often respond similarly to similar stimuli. The further the test stimulus is from the original S+, the weaker the response (seen in pigeons trained with light colors). Discrimination: Animals learn to distinguish between stimuli that signal different outcomes: S+ (positive discriminative stimulus): Signals reinforcement. S– (negative discriminative stimulus): Signals no reinforcement. Example: A child behaves better when parents are around (S+) than when they're not (S–).} \tn % Row Count 12 (+ 12) % Row 19 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Complex Discriminations Animals (like pigeons) can make surprisingly complex discriminations: Water vs. non-water pictures Trees vs. non-trees Recognizing individual humans from varied angles Shows learning goes beyond simple sensory cues and includes abstract categories.} \tn % Row Count 18 (+ 6) % Row 20 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Shaping (Successive Approximations) Used to teach complex or unlikely behaviors. Reinforcement is given step-by-step as the animal's behavior gradually approximates the desired action. Example: To teach a rat to press a high lever: Reinforce being near the lever → facing it → raising head → touching lever → pressing lever.} \tn % Row Count 25 (+ 7) % Row 21 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{What is a Reinforcer? Primary Reinforcers: Naturally rewarding (e.g., food, water). Social Reinforcers: Praise, smiles, etc. Conditioned Reinforcers: Gain value by being associated with primary reinforcers (e.g., money). Some reinforcers are informational or experiential (e.g., watching a toy train, using a wheel).} \tn % Row Count 32 (+ 7) \end{tabularx} \par\addvspace{1.3em} \vfill \columnbreak \begin{tabularx}{8.4cm}{X} \SetRowColor{DarkBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Operant/Instrumental Conditioning (cont)}} \tn % Row 22 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Behavioral Contrast The effectiveness of a reinforcer depends on context and past experience. A reward may seem large or small depending on what the subject was used to before. Example: 16 food pellets feel small after 60 but generous after 4.} \tn % Row Count 5 (+ 5) % Row 23 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Intrinsic Motivation and Overjustification Effect Overjustification Effect: External rewards can reduce intrinsic interest in an activity. Example: Children who initially liked drawing became less interested after being rewarded with "Good Player" certificates and then having those rewards removed.} \tn % Row Count 12 (+ 7) % Row 24 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Schedules of Reinforcement} \tn % Row Count 13 (+ 1) % Row 25 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Partial Reinforcement: Behavior is reinforced only sometimes, not every time. Effect: Leads to greater resistance to extinction (we keep trying even if not always rewarded).} \tn % Row Count 17 (+ 4) % Row 26 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Types of Reinforcement Schedules Ratio Schedules (Based on number of responses) Fixed Ratio (FR): Reinforcement after a set number of responses. E.g., FR 2 → reward after every 2 responses. Variable Ratio (VR): Reinforcement after a varying number of responses (average-based). E.g., VR 10 → reward on average after 10 responses (might be 5, then 15, etc.). Common in gambling (e.g., slot machines). Interval Schedules (Based on passage of time) Fixed Interval (FI): First response after a fixed time is rewarded. E.g., FI 3 minutes → reward given after 3 minutes if response made. Variable Interval (VI): Time interval changes; average time determines reward schedule. E.g., VI 8 minutes → reward on average after 8 minutes.} \tn % Row Count 32 (+ 15) \end{tabularx} \par\addvspace{1.3em} \vfill \columnbreak \begin{tabularx}{8.4cm}{X} \SetRowColor{DarkBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Operant/Instrumental Conditioning (cont)}} \tn % Row 27 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Contingency in Instrumental Conditioning} \tn % Row Count 1 (+ 1) % Row 28 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Contingency: Behavior must predict the reward—not just follow it. Similar to classical conditioning: Prediction, not just pairing, is key. The likelihood of reward must be greater with the behavior than without it. 🔧 Control Matters: Organisms like having control over outcomes. When individuals can predict and influence rewards, learning is enhanced.} \tn % Row Count 9 (+ 8) % Row 29 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Experiment: Infants and Mobiles} \tn % Row Count 10 (+ 1) % Row 30 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Group 1: Infants could control the mobile by moving their heads. Result: Enjoyed and engaged with the mobile. Group 2: Mobile moved independently, not due to the infant's action. Result: Infants lost interest. Key Point: Even 2-month-old infants prefer control and enjoy mastery.} \tn % Row Count 16 (+ 6) % Row 31 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Learned Helplessness} \tn % Row Count 17 (+ 1) % Row 32 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{The Dog Study: Group A: Dogs could stop shocks by pressing a panel. Group B: Received same shocks, but had no control—shocks were inescapable. Later task: Jump a barrier to avoid shock. Group A: Learned and escaped quickly. Group B: Became passive, didn't try to escape—even though it was possible.} \tn % Row Count 24 (+ 7) % Row 33 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Learned Helplessness: When previous lack of control leads to a belief that future attempts are useless. Leads to passivity, even when escape or success is possible.} \tn % Row Count 28 (+ 4) \hhline{>{\arrayrulecolor{DarkBackground}}-} \end{tabularx} \par\addvspace{1.3em} \begin{tabularx}{8.4cm}{X} \SetRowColor{DarkBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Latent Learning}} \tn % Row 0 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Edward Tolman: Believed learning is more than behavior change—it's acquiring knowledge.} \tn % Row Count 2 (+ 2) % Row 1 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Latent Learning:} \tn % Row Count 3 (+ 1) % Row 2 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Learning that occurs without any obvious reinforcement and does not immediately manifest in behavior. The learned knowledge becomes apparent only when there is motivation to demonstrate it.} \tn % Row Count 7 (+ 4) % Row 3 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Classic Experiment (Tolman \& Honzik, 1930):} \tn % Row Count 8 (+ 1) % Row 4 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Rats explored a maze for 10 days with no reward → no visible change in behavior. On Day 11, food was introduced at the goal box → rats quickly and accurately ran to the food. This showed they had formed a mental map of the maze during unrewarded exploration.} \tn % Row Count 14 (+ 6) % Row 5 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Mental Maps (Cognitive Maps):} \tn % Row Count 15 (+ 1) % Row 6 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Internal representations of the environment. Allow organisms to navigate spaces efficiently even without direct reinforcement.} \tn % Row Count 18 (+ 3) % Row 7 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Implications} \tn % Row Count 19 (+ 1) % Row 8 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Learning ≠ Immediate Behavior Change: Just because behavior hasn't changed doesn't mean learning hasn't happened. Supports Cognitive Perspective: Emphasizes the role of internal cognitive processes, not just stimulus-response links. Challenge to Behaviorism: Opposes the strict behaviorist view (e.g., Thorndike's law of effect) that learning only occurs via reinforcement. Practical Relevance: Students may learn a lot during lectures without immediately demonstrating it. Skills and knowledge can emerge when they become relevant or useful. Used by Many Species: Not limited to humans—many animals develop mental maps for foraging, navigation, etc.} \tn % Row Count 33 (+ 14) \hhline{>{\arrayrulecolor{DarkBackground}}-} \end{tabularx} \par\addvspace{1.3em} \begin{tabularx}{8.4cm}{X} \SetRowColor{DarkBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Observational Learning}} \tn % Row 0 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{What is Observational Learning?} \tn % Row Count 1 (+ 1) % Row 1 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Learning by watching others and imitating their behavior.} \tn % Row Count 3 (+ 2) % Row 2 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Also called social learning or vicarious learning.} \tn % Row Count 4 (+ 1) % Row 3 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{No direct experience or reinforcement needed—just observation.} \tn % Row Count 6 (+ 2) % Row 4 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Once thought to be uniquely human, but now observed in many animals too. 📚 Examples: Monkeys: Learn fear by watching another monkey react fearfully. Pigeons: Imitate behaviors like pecking or stepping to get rewards after watching others.} \tn % Row Count 11 (+ 5) % Row 5 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Mirror Neurons} \tn % Row Count 12 (+ 1) % Row 6 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Special neurons in the frontal lobe near the motor cortex. Fire when you perform an action and when you see someone else perform the same action. Help with understanding others' actions and imitating them. Found in many species, including humans.} \tn % Row Count 18 (+ 6) % Row 7 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Human Imitation Starts Early} \tn % Row Count 19 (+ 1) % Row 8 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Infants imitate facial expressions within the first month of life. Later, they mimic a wide range of behaviors. Two types of imitation: Mimicry: Copying the exact behavior. Modeling: Learning general rules or what behavior is "okay" in a setting.} \tn % Row Count 24 (+ 5) % Row 9 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Deferred imitation: means imitating an action after a delay, not right after seeing it. The behavior is observed first, then reproduced later, sometimes hours, days, or even weeks afterward.} \tn % Row Count 28 (+ 4) % Row 10 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Bandura's Bobo Doll Experiment} \tn % Row Count 29 (+ 1) % Row 11 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Albert Bandura wanted to test whether children learn aggressive behavior by observing adults.} \tn % Row Count 31 (+ 2) \end{tabularx} \par\addvspace{1.3em} \vfill \columnbreak \begin{tabularx}{8.4cm}{X} \SetRowColor{DarkBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Observational Learning (cont)}} \tn % Row 12 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Experiment Setup} \tn % Row Count 1 (+ 1) % Row 13 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Participants: Preschool children (around 3–6 years old) Groups: Aggressive model: Kids watched an adult physically and verbally attack a Bobo doll (e.g., hitting, kicking, saying "pow!"). Non-aggressive model: Kids watched an adult play quietly and nicely with toys. Control group: Kids saw no model.} \tn % Row Count 8 (+ 7) % Row 14 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Results} \tn % Row Count 9 (+ 1) % Row 15 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{After watching the adult, kids were taken to a room with toys including a Bobo doll. Children who saw the aggressive model were more likely to imitate the aggression — even using the same actions and words. Some children went beyond imitation, showing new aggressive behaviors.} \tn % Row Count 15 (+ 6) % Row 16 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Key Findings} \tn % Row Count 16 (+ 1) % Row 17 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Children learn social behavior like aggression through observation, not just direct reinforcement. Modeling matters: kids imitate what they see, especially if the model is powerful or similar to them. This learning can be immediate or delayed (deferred imitation). Boys showed more physical aggression than girls, but both imitated the behavior.} \tn % Row Count 23 (+ 7) % Row 18 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{4 Steps of Observational Learning} \tn % Row Count 24 (+ 1) % Row 19 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Attention: You have to notice the behaviour} \tn % Row Count 25 (+ 1) % Row 20 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Retention: You have to remember the behaviour} \tn % Row Count 26 (+ 1) % Row 21 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Reproduction: You must be able to replicate it} \tn % Row Count 27 (+ 1) % Row 22 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Motivation: You must {\emph{want}} to do it} \tn % Row Count 28 (+ 1) % Row 23 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Vicarious reinforcement: Vicarious reinforcement is when we learn by watching someone else get rewarded for a behavior — and then we're more likely to do that behavior ourselves.} \tn % Row Count 32 (+ 4) \end{tabularx} \par\addvspace{1.3em} \vfill \columnbreak \begin{tabularx}{8.4cm}{X} \SetRowColor{DarkBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{\bf\textcolor{white}{Observational Learning (cont)}} \tn % Row 24 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Characteristics of the Model That Affect Learning:} \tn % Row Count 1 (+ 1) % Row 25 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Perceived Similarity People are more likely to imitate models who are similar to them (in age, gender, interests, etc.).} \tn % Row Count 4 (+ 3) % Row 26 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Perceived Competence If the model appears skilled or knowledgeable, observers are more likely to imitate them.} \tn % Row Count 7 (+ 3) % Row 27 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Status and Prestige Models with high social status (celebrities, teachers, respected peers) are more influential.} \tn % Row Count 10 (+ 3) % Row 28 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Warmth and Nurturance Models who are kind, friendly, and caring tend to be imitated more often.} \tn % Row Count 12 (+ 2) % Row 29 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Power or Authority Models who hold authority or power (like parents or police officers) can strongly influence behavior.} \tn % Row Count 15 (+ 3) % Row 30 \SetRowColor{LightBackground} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Consistency of Behavior Consistent behavior across situations makes a model more trustworthy and worth copying.} \tn % Row Count 18 (+ 3) % Row 31 \SetRowColor{white} \mymulticolumn{1}{x{8.4cm}}{Reinforcement or Punishment Observed If the model is rewarded, the observer is more likely to imitate. If the model is punished, the behavior is less likely to be copied.} \tn % Row Count 22 (+ 4) \hhline{>{\arrayrulecolor{DarkBackground}}-} \end{tabularx} \par\addvspace{1.3em} % That's all folks \end{multicols*} \end{document}